
Diabetes risk factors much higher in young
Sri Lankans
Scientists
at King's College London and the National Diabetes Centre (Sri Lanka)
have found evidence of a high number of risk factors for type 2 diabetes
among the young urban population in Sri Lanka. The study is the first
large-scale investigation into diabetes risk among children and young
people in South Asia, and provides further evidence that the region is
rapidly becoming a hotspot in the growing international diabetes
epidemic.
The study, published in the journal PLoS One, is part of a research
program aiming at developing methods to prevent diabetes in young people
in Sri Lanka, as the disease is now having a major public health impact.
The scientists suggest that urgent action is now required to raise
awareness of diabetes and obesity in developing countries and encourage
young people to make lifestyle changes to reduce their risk.
According to the World Health Organisation (WHO), 346 million people
worldwide have diabetes, with 80 percent of diabetes deaths occurring in
low- and middle-income countries. Recent research has shown that urban
populations in South Asia are increasingly at risk from developing type
2 diabetes, which develops largely as a result of excess body weight and
physical inactivity. In Sri Lanka, studies have shown that one in five
adults has either diabetes or pre-diabetes, but until now no research
has been carried out into risk-factors among young people. The DIABRISK-SL
project is an international collaboration between scientists in Sri
Lanka, led by Dr Mahen Wijesuriya and the UK, led by Dr Janaka
Karalliedde from the Cardiovascular Division at King's College London.
The team surveyed 22,507 people aged between 10 and 40 from cities in
Sri Lanka to check for various early risk factors for type 2 diabetes -
such as high body mass index (BMI), raised waist circumference and high
levels of physical inactivity. They also checked for family history of
the disease.
The survey revealed that 5,163 people (23 percent) had two or more
risk factors for diabetes, with two or more risk factors found in 24
percent of children aged 10-14. Raised BMI was found in nearly 20
percent of children aged 10-14, and 15 percent of children aged 15-19.
Most worryingly, the prevalence of physical inactivity and central
obesity was nearly 40p.c. in females aged under 16. The results also
showed that physical inactivity was a lot higher among females in all
age groups, with overall inactivity rising in both sexes with age.
'What we have found in this report really confirms that South Asia is
becoming the centre of a worldwide diabetes epidemic,' said Dr
Karalliedde.
'We were expecting the levels of risk factors to be high, but we were
still surprised at just how high they were.
The fact that we found such a high prevalence in children has not
been shown Sri Lanka before, or anywhere else in South Asia, and is of
great concern.
'This dramatic rise is clearly linked to a decline in physical
activity and mirrors global trends of rising childhood obesity. Being
overweight in childhood means people are much more likely to become
obese as adults and will have a greater risk of developing type 2
diabetes and cardiovascular disease.
Now that we know how widespread the problem is, we can take steps
towards identifying high risk groups to evolving preventive strategies.'
Dr Mahen Wijesuriya, of the National Diabetes Centre, said: 'These data
highlight the need for early intervention in younger people in Sri
Lanka.
A primary prevention intervention trial is now under way to evaluate
the effects of intensive lifestyle intervention on improving diet and
exercise.
We will have the results of this in the next two years.' Dr
Wijesuriya emphasised the importance of public health education and
awareness and stressed that these results have already contributed to
the development of a National Non-Communicable Disease Strategy to
combat type 2 diabetes in Sri Lanka.
Prof Jean Claude Mbanya, President of the International Diabetes
Federation, said: 'These figures reflect the disturbing rise in risk
factors for type 2 diabetes among young people being seen worldwide.
This is an example of good-quality scientific research that will
bring solutions to the global epidemic of diabetes and other chronic
non-communicable disease.
'We hope that the DIABRISK-SL project in Sri Lanka will lead to
effective and cost-effective interventions that work in the real world.
This is a golden opportunity to make a very deep and very positive
long-term impact on individuals, families and entire communities in Sri
Lanka.'
MNT
The meaning of spoken words understood by 6-9-month-olds
At an age when "ba-ba" and "da-da" may be their only utterances,
infants nevertheless comprehend words for many common objects, according
to a new study.
In
research focused on 6-to-9-month-old babies, psychologists Elika
Bergelson and Daniel Swingley demonstrated that the infants learned the
meanings of words for foods and body parts through their daily
experience with language.
These findings unseat a previously held consensus about infant
learning.
It was widely believed that infants between six and nine months,
while able to perceive and understand elements of the sounds of their
native language, did not yet possess the ability to grasp the meanings
Comprehension though speech.
Most psychologists believed word comprehension didn't emerge until
closer to a child's first birthday.
In fact, infants are often referred to as "pre-linguistic," according
to Bergelson. But there have been few attempts to determine just when
infants begin understanding what is meant by specific words. The belief
that infants do not comprehend language for most of the first year is
easy to understand, given that infants do not often speak in words, or
even gesture meaningfully, before 10 or 11 months.
To test this belief, the researchers recruited caregivers to bring
their children to a lab to complete two different kinds of test. In the
first, a child sat on the caregiver's lap facing a screen on which there
were images of one food item and one body part.
The caregiver wore headphones and heard a statement such as, "Look at
the apple," or, "Where's the apple?" and then repeated it to the child.
The caregiver also wore a visor to avoid seeing the screen. An
eye-tracking device, which can distinguish precisely where a child is
looking and when, then followed the child's gaze.
The second kind of test had the same set-up, except that, instead of
the screen displaying a food item and a body part, it displayed objects
in natural contexts, such as a few foods laid out on a table, or a human
figure. For both kinds of test, the question was whether hearing a word
for something on the screen would lead children to look at that object
more, indicating that they understood the word. In total, 33
6-to-9-month olds were tested. The researchers also had 50 children from
10 to 20 months complete the same tests to see how their abilities
compared with the younger group. As part of their analysis, the
researchers corrected for eye movements not related to caregivers'
speech.
"So if you have a boring cup and a really colourful cup, they're
going to look at the more interesting thing, all else being equal."
To eliminate this potential source of error, the researchers
subtracted the amount of time that the babies gazed at a given object
when it was not being named from the time they looked when it was named.
In both the two-picture and scene tests, the researchers found that
the 6- to 9-month-old babies fixed their gaze more on the picture that
was named than on the other image or images, indicating that they
understood that the word was associated with the appropriate object.
This is the first demonstration that children of this age can
understand such words.
"There had been a few demonstrations of understanding before,
involving words such as mommy and daddy," Swingley said. "Our study is
different in looking at more generic words, words that refer to
categories."
"We're testing things that look different every time you see them,"
Bergelson said. "There's some variety in apples and noses, and 'nose'
doesn't just mean your nose; it could mean anybody's nose. This is one
of the things that makes word learning complicated: words often refer to
categories, not just individuals."The researchers were also curious to
know whether they could observe a pattern of learning during the months
from 6 to 9.
But, when they compared the performance of 6 and 7-month-old babies
with that of 8- and 9-month olds, they found no improvements. Factoring
in the results of the older babies, the researchers found little
improvement until the children reached roughly 14 months, at which point
word recognition jumped markedly.
The study's novel results contribute to an ongoing debate about
infant language acquisition and cognitive development.
- psychone.net
Motivation to exercise affects behaviour
For many people, the motivation to exercise fluctuates from week to
week, and these fluctuations predict whether they will be physically
active, according to researchers at Penn State. In an effort to
understand how the motivation to exercise is linked to behavior, the
researchers examined college students' intentions to be physically
active as well as their actual activity levels.
"Many
of us set New Year's resolutions to be more physically active, and we
expect these resolutions to be stable throughout the year," said David
Conroy, professor of kinesiology. "One of the things we see in this
study is that from week to week our motivation can change a lot, and
these weekly changes in motivation can be destructive to our
resolutions."
Conroy and colleagues recruited 33 college students and assessed over
a ten-week period both the students' weekly intentions to be physically
active and their activity levels. During each of the 10 weeks,
participants were instructed to log on to a website and to rate their
intentions to perform physical activity for the week ahead. To assess
physical activity, participants were instructed to wear pedometers each
day for the first four weeks.
The team found that for many of the participants, the motivation to
exercise fluctuated on a weekly basis, and these fluctuations were
linked to their behavior. The results appear in the current issue of the
Journal of Sport ; Exercise Psychology.
"Our motivation to be physically active changes on a weekly basis
because we have so many demands on our time," said Conroy. "Maybe one
week we're sick or we have a work deadline - or, in the case of
students, an upcoming examination. But these lapses in motivation really
seem to be destructive. Our results suggest that people with
consistently strong intentions to exercise have the best chance of
actually following through on their intentions, while people with the
greatest fluctuations in their motivation have the hardest time using
that motivation to regulate their behaviour."
According to Amanda Hyde, graduate student in kinesiology, the latter
group may still be successful at incorporating physical activity into
their lives.
"Maybe the way to get these people to be more physical active isn't
necessarily by increasing their motivation" she said, " but rather by
changing the way they do things in their lives so exercise automatically
fits within their schedule, like walking to work rather than driving or
taking the stairs rather than the elevator."
Conroy added that consistency of intentions is not the only thing
that matters in predicting whether or not a person will be active. It
also matters if it is a weekday or the weekend.
"We saw that people who consistently reported stronger intentions to
be active were more active during the week, but then on weekends the
pattern flipped for them," said Conroy. "If a person was really
motivated during the week, then he or she crashed on the weekend."
Conroy said that people seem to have different systems that motivate
their behavior during the week and on the weekend. "We speculate that
this reflects the fact that college students are in the midst of a
transition that significantly increases their autonomy," said Conroy. -sciencedaily.com
Scientist works to detach protein that HIV uses as protective shield
One
of the frustrations for scientists working on HIV/AIDS treatment has
been the human immunodeficiency virus' ability to evade the body's
immune system. Now an Indiana University researcher has discovered a
compound that could help put the immune system back in the hunt.It's not
that the human immune system doesn't recognise HIV.
Indeed, an infection causes the body to unleash antibodies that
attack the virus, and initially some HIV is destroyed.
But HIV is able to quickly defend itself by co-opting a part of the
innate human immune system - the immune system people are born with,
called the complement. The complement includes a vital mechanism that
prevents immune system cells from attacking the body's own cells. HIV is
able to incorporate a key protein in that self-protection mechanism,
CD59, and by doing so makes itself appear to be one of the body's normal
cells, not an infective agent.
In laboratories at the Indiana University School of Medicine, Andy
Qigui Yu, is testing a promising compound that may counteract HIV's
ability to hijack the immune system's protection mechanism.
"HIV is very clever. As it replicates inside cells, it takes on the
CD59. The virus is covered with CD59, so the immune system treats the
virus like your own normal cells," Dr. Yu said.
The new grant will support not only Dr. Yu's research into compounds
that may block the ability of HIV to hide behind the CD59 "cloak," but
also his work to identify the mechanism the virus uses to incorporate
CD59.
"If we find that mechanism, then we can develop something to block
that incorporation, and HIV may lose that protection from the immune
system," Dr. Yu said.
- sciencedaily.com
Tracking down cause of birth defect
A
research team has pinpointed the source of a genetic disorder that
causes life-threatening birth defects, which may allow doctors to
quickly diagnose and better treat the disease.
Babies born with the disorder, known as Loeys-Dietz syndrome or
Marfan syndrome type II, have cleft palates and other facial
characteristics similar to babies born with other diseases - but also
happen to suffer potentially fatal heart defects, making it critical for
them to receive an accurate diagnosis right away.
Researchers from USC found an abnormally high amount of a protein
known as Transforming Growth Factor Beta (TGF-â) outside of cells -
which may be revealed by a blood or tissue test - in patients with
characteristic facial defects is a key indicator of Loeys-Dietz. "If we
can screen patients for this, it can identify Loeys-Dietz syndrome and
inform clinical practice," said Yang Chai, director for the USC Center
for Craniofacial Molecular Biology and corresponding author of the
study.
"And perhaps, one day we can manipulate the amount [of TGF-â] and
possibly rescue the cleft palate before a baby is born. The prospects of
this are very promising."
Led by senior post-doctoral fellow Junichi Iwata of the Ostrow School
of Dentistry at USC, researchers made their discovery by studying the
foetal development of mice. They found that mutations that affect the
way TGF-â communicates outside of a cell may cause Loeys-Dietz syndrome.
TGF-â controls many of the functions within a cell, and is known to
be heavily involved in the palate's formation - or failure to form.
Typically, it uses a receptor protein known as TGFBR2 to communicate
outside of the cell. However, if a mutation causes a roadblock on that
communication highway, TGF-â may rely on surface streets to get its
signal out.
In clinical studies, the activation of this separate signalling
pathway resulted in palate and facial defects akin to Loeys-Dietz
syndrome. A telltale sign of the alternate pathway's activity is an
abnormally high amount of TGF-â outside of the cell.
- MNT
|