-new-navin-3.jpg)
Wild birds learn foreign 'languages'
Birds may be bilingual, trilingual or better, suggest new findings
that birds in the wild can learn the vocalizations of other species.
The discovery not only proves that birds eavesdrop (listen in) on
what other birds are saying, but it also provides some of the strongest
evidence to date that birds can learn "foreign" calls, as opposed to
just confusing similar sounds with their own.
While humans may learn a foreign language for work or pleasure, the
skill can mean life or death for little songbirds that, according to the
study, pay attention to the alarm calls sounded by other birds when a
predator, such as a hawk, approaches.
"It's tricky to know what goes on inside another species' head," lead
author Robert Magrath told Discovery News. "At one extreme, perhaps they
are labelling, such as 'flying hawk approaching at 10m!' or 'hawk flying
by in the distance,' or 'predator on the ground,' etc."
 |
A honeyeater,
one of the birds that were
subjected to the study |
Magrath, an associate professor of botany and zoology at the
Australian National University, added that the vocalizations could be
prompted by anxiety too.
"The best evidence is that both labelling and fear have a role," he
said.
Magrath and colleagues Benjamin Pitcher and Janet Gardner studied
three Australian birds: superb fairy-wrens, white-browed scrubwrens, and
New Holland honeyeaters. He prompted each to sound an alarm call using a
gliding model sparrowhawk. This predatory bird has a taste for
fairy-wrens and scrubwrens. The scrubwren alarm call sounds similar to
that of the fairy-wren, so avian experts previously thought the birds
simply thought the sounds came from one of their own.
To test this possibility, the researchers played scrubwren calls to
fairy-wrens at Canberra, where these two wren populations overlap, and
at Macquarie Marshes, where the two species do not live close to each
other and therefore have no chance to learn each others' calls.
Sure enough, the Canberra birds did as they were "told" when played
the other species' alarm calls, which was to flee pronto (immediately).
The Macquarie Marsh birds did next to zilch (nothing), suggesting they
hadn't learned the other species' calls.
To further test their abilities, the scientists played the alarm
calls of New Holland honeyeaters to the wrens. The honeyeater
vocalizations sound nothing at all like the wren calls.
Once again, the wrens demonstrated that they'd learned the meaning of
other species' calls, since they fled when the honeyeater alarm sounded.
This study, published in the latest Proceedings of the Royal Society
B, is unusual in that the researchers used alarm calls which, Magrath
said, "are very short and everything happens quickly." These calls are
intentionally brief and high-pitched, making them difficult for hawks
and other predators to hear.
Birds seem to understand certain other birds so long as learning
through association or social means is possible. They may even
understand some mammals and rodents, such as small antelopes and
squirrels, which appear to learn bird sounds as well.
- Discovery Channel
|